Читаем Shadows of Forgotten Ancestors полностью

The cooperation of the cells in stromatolites and other colonial organisms can be understood as selfish at the level of the gene, since they’re all close relatives. Cooperation of the chloroplast and the cell with which it forms a symbiotic attachment—is this selfish? The cell that eats its chloroplasts is at a competitive disadvantage. It refrains from eating them not because it has even a glimmer of altruistic feeling for the chloroplasts, but because it’s dead without them. It forgoes the pleasures of a chloroplast meal for a substantial future benefit. It exercises restraint on short-term, selfish behavior. It practices impulse control. Selfishness still prevails, but we are made aware of the distinction between short- and long-term selfishness.

For most social animals, and for obvious reasons, the animals you grow up with tend to be close relatives. So if you cooperate, if you show what superficially might seem like altruism, it’s naturally directed toward close kin and can therefore be explained as kin selection. An organism might forego its own replication, for example, and devote its life to improving the chances of the survival and reproduction of close relatives—those with very similar DNA sequences. If all that counts is which sequences will be widely represented in the life of the future, those species with a flair for altruism might do well. They can help ensure that much of their genetic information is passed on, even if none of their atoms wind up in the bodies of the next generation.14

The geneticist R. A. Fisher described heroism as a predisposition inclining its bearer toward “an increased probability of entering an occupation not easily to be reconciled with family life.” Nevertheless, Fisher argued, heroism—in humans or in other animals—might carry a selective advantage by preserving the very similar genetic sequences of close relatives, enabling such sequences to be passed on to future generations. This is one of the first clear articulations of kin selection. Parents sacrificing themselves for a child can be understood on similar grounds. The hero or the devoted parent will be doing simply what feels “right,” without attempting any calculus of benefit versus risk to the gene pool. But the reason it feels “right,” Fisher proposed, is that extended families characterized by conscientious parenting and heroes aplenty will tend to do very well.*

Animals may be willing to make real sacrifices for close relatives, but not for more distant kin. Think of it this way: Imagine sleeping soundly at night, knowing that your children are starving, homeless, or gravely ill. For almost all of us, it would be unthinkable. But forty thousand children die each day of easily preventable hunger, neglect, or disease. Institutions such as the United Nations Children’s Fund are in place that could save these children—with innoculations against illness, with a few cents a day worth of salts and sugar. But the money is unavailable. Other needs are deemed more pressing. The children continue to die while we sleep well. They are far away. They are not ours. Now tell us you don’t believe in the reality of kin selection.

Still, if you find yourself among others of your own species who are not your near kin, surely it is to your advantage to cooperate against a common enemy. You can draw upon behavior evolved for kin selection in order that a group of animals not closely related can cohere and survive.* And if altruism is one of your talents, you might find yourself practicing it even on animals of another species. Dogs are known to risk their lives to save humans—surely no close relatives. Nor does the hope of future reward explain their behavior.

How are we to understand well-attested cases of dolphins saving drowning humans by repeatedly nuzzling them up to the surface and pushing them toward shore? Is the dolphin unable to distinguish the thrashing human from an infant dolphin in trouble? This is highly unlikely; dolphins are discerning observers. What about cases of abandoned or strayed human infants being raised by wolf mothers that have lost their pups, or birds of a different species brooding cuckoo eggs? Why do drivers swerve to avoid hitting a dog on the road, although they thereby put their own children in the back seat at increased risk? What about youngsters dashing back into the burning house to rescue the cat? Such cases of courage and care directed to other species may derive from a misdirected kin selection, but they do happen and they do save lives. Shouldn’t we then expect to find altruistic behavior much more frequently directed toward other members of the same species, even if they’re not close relatives?

Перейти на страницу:

Похожие книги

100 великих загадок Африки
100 великих загадок Африки

Африка – это не только вечное наследие Древнего Египта и магическое искусство негритянских народов, не только снега Килиманджаро, слоны и пальмы. Из этой книги, которую составил профессиональный африканист Николай Непомнящий, вы узнаете – в документально точном изложении – захватывающие подробности поисков пиратских кладов и леденящие душу свидетельства тех, кто уцелел среди бесчисленных опасностей, подстерегающих путешественника в Африке. Перед вами предстанет сверкающий экзотическими красками мир африканских чудес: таинственные фрески ныне пустынной Сахары и легендарные бриллианты; целый народ, живущий в воде озера Чад, и племя двупалых людей; негритянские волшебники и маги…

Николай Николаевич Непомнящий

Приключения / Научная литература / Путешествия и география / Прочая научная литература / Образование и наука
Агрессия
Агрессия

Конрад Лоренц (1903-1989) — выдающийся австрийский учёный, лауреат Нобелевской премии, один из основоположников этологии, науки о поведении животных.В данной книге автор прослеживает очень интересные аналогии в поведении различных видов позвоночных и вида Homo sapiens, именно поэтому книга публикуется в серии «Библиотека зарубежной психологии».Утверждая, что агрессивность является врождённым, инстинктивно обусловленным свойством всех высших животных — и доказывая это на множестве убедительных примеров, — автор подводит к выводу;«Есть веские основания считать внутривидовую агрессию наиболее серьёзной опасностью, какая грозит человечеству в современных условиях культурноисторического и технического развития.»На русском языке публиковались книги К. Лоренца: «Кольцо царя Соломона», «Человек находит друга», «Год серого гуся».

Вячеслав Владимирович Шалыгин , Конрад Захариас Лоренц , Конрад Лоренц , Маргарита Епатко

Фантастика / Самиздат, сетевая литература / Научная литература / Ужасы и мистика / Прочая научная литература / Образование и наука / Ужасы