Well, there are markings on the walls. Spirals, circles. Writing, I suppose. God knows what it says. There's an elevated work surface in the room, but nothing that looks like a chair.
"Sitting is overrated."
Yeah, perhaps. I'm standing myself. It's all very — the door! The door is opening, crumpling aside like an accordion, and—
"Yes? Yes? What do you see?"
Hello? Hello! Um, my name is Jake. Jake Sullivan.
"What do you see? What do they look like?"
I guess we'll have to learn each other's language, eh? That's okay…
"Jake! What do they look like?"
We're going to have some interesting times together, I can see that…
"Jake? Jake?"
Like I said, my name is Jake, and I guess I'm here to tell you a little bit about what it means to be human.
There was a pause, presumably while the other me thought things that weren't articulated in words, then:
But, you know, I'm in contact with somebody else, and I think he knows even more about being human than I do. Let's see what he has to say…
FURTHER READING
Consciousness is back, baby! For most of the twentieth century, brain studies avoided any discussion of consciousness — the feeling of subjective experience, the apprehension of qualia, the sense that it is like something to be you or me. But in the last decade, the issue of consciousness has very much moved to center stage in the exploration of the human brain.
Although I touched on the nature of consciousness in my 1995 novel The Terminal Experiment, and again in 1998's Factoring Humanity, I find myself drawn back to this fertile ground once more, in large part because consciousness studies are so multidisciplinary — and I firmly believe it's the interplay of disparate elements that makes for good science fiction. Whereas twenty years ago, you'd be hard-pressed to find any academic talking seriously about consciousness, these days quantum physicists, evolutionary psychologists, neuroscientists, artificial-intelligence researchers, philosophers, and even lowly novelists are engaged in the debate.
(Indeed, one could argue that novelists were the only ones who took consciousness seriously for much of the last century: we strove, however ineffectually, to capture the stream of consciousness in our narratives, and to explore the limitations and richness of constrained points-of-view and subjective experience … all while the Skinnerian behaviorists were telling the world that such things were meaningless.)
The resurgent interest in consciousness is perhaps best summed up by the existence of the essential Journal of Consciousness Studies, published by Imprint Academic.
JCS is subtitled "Controversies in Science and the Humanities," and refers to itself as "an international multidisciplinary journal." You can learn more about it at www.imprint.co.uk/jcs.
I own a complete set of this journal, which is now in its twelfth year, and consulted it extensively while writing Mindscan. However, the papers in it are often very technical; for those interested in popular discussions of consciousness, I recommend the following books, which also influenced me while I was working on this novel.
Carter, Rita. Exploring Consciousness. Berkeley: University of California Press, 2002. An excellent introduction.
Carter, Rita. Mapping the Mind. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1998. A good overview of how the brain works.
Crick, Francis. The Astonishing Hypothesis: The Scientific Search for the Soul.
New York: Charles Scribner's Sons, 1994. Crick — the co-discoverer of the helical structure of DNA — believed that consciousness didn't really exist.
Dennett, Daniel C. Consciousness Explained. New York: Little Brown, 1991. Often referred to by those who think there's something special about human self-awareness as "Consciousness Explained Away."
Freeman, Anthony. Consciousness: A Guide to the Debates. Santa Barbara, California: ABC-CLIO, 2003. A fascinating look at the various controversies.